In a recent study, researcher Yuko Munakata tested the self-regulation and self-control abilities of children in two cultures: The U.S. and Japan. She published the research in the empirical journal Psychological Science, but also wrote an accessible description of it for the popular online magazine, Scientific American. The heading wrote, A classic test of self-control can carry complex cultural biases.
In case you are unfamiliar with the marshmallow test, here is Dr. Munakata's description of it:
The classic marshmallow test, invented by the late psychologist Walter Mischel, involves presenting a child with one marshmallow and explaining that they can have it now or they can have two later if they wait until the marshmallow giver returns. Children sit alone in a room with the tempting treat. Psychologists have generally viewed the length of time kids resist the sweet as a measure of their self-control: how effectively they can inhibit impulsive behaviors and work toward longer-term goals.
In their study, the research team compared marshmallow waiting time among four- and five-year-old children in two cultures. Here's a description of part of the study:
We worked with 144 children in both the U.S. and Japan. We first ensured that all of these four- and five-year-olds had eaten marshmallows before. We asked parents how often their children waited to eat until others were served and how well their children could suppress impulsive behaviors.
Then the kids faced the classic test: Did they want one marshmallow now or two later? Most children in the U.S. waited less than four minutes before tasting the one marshmallow. Most children in Japan waited for two marshmallows for the maximum possible time—15 minutes!
But the study did not stop there:
If we had stopped there, we might have simply concluded that Japanese kids have better self-control. But we conducted another test. We presented children with a wrapped gift and told them that they could open it now or they could have two gifts if they waited. The pattern flipped. Most children in Japan waited less than five minutes before unwrapping the gift, while most children in the U.S. waited the maximum 15 minutes or close to it.
In a moment, we'll go over the researchers' reasoning about why they found this cultural difference. But for now, see if you can identify the features of this factorial design.
a) Take a moment to classify the variables. There are three of them!
Variable name | What are the variable's levels? |
Is this manipulated or measured? | Is this an IV, PV*, or a DV? | For IVs and PVs: is it within groups or independent groups? |
*PV means participant variable--it is treated as an IV, but it is not actually manipulated. Examples of PVs are demographic variables such as gender, ethnicity, or class year.
b) This was a factorial design. What kind? Your answer should read ___ x ___, and mention whether it was an independent groups factorial, a within-groups factorial, or a mixed factorial design (hint: to decide, look back at how you coded the IVs and PVs on the table.
c) See if you can sketch the results the author described. You can choose a bar graph or a line graph. Remember to put the DV on the y-axis, one IV/PV on the x-axis, and the other IV/PV as a line or bar color.
d) Do you think this factorial design's results found an interaction? If so, describe it in a sentence or two.
Now let's read the researcher's explanation for why they that cultural differences in self-control depended on the task used:
What was going on? It seems that with repeated experiences delaying gratification, children can develop habits that make it easier to wait in the future. According to the reports from their parents, the Japanese children had stronger habits of waiting to eat than the U.S. children. The stronger these habits, the longer the children waited for two marshmallows. But when it comes to opening gifts, children in the U.S. likely experience waiting more consistently. Birthday presents can sit on a table, unopened, until the end of a party. Christmas presents may sit under a tree for days before they can be unwrapped on December 25. In Japan, people give gifts year-round on simple occasions that do not involve traditions of waiting.
This result was fascinating to me as a cultural psychologist! Turns out there was no "culture main effect"--there's no overall difference in self control between U.S. and Japanese children. Instead, culture interacts with task, so kids do better at the self-control task they practice more in their culture.